LSE - Small Logo
LSE - Small Logo

Berno Buechel

Lydia Mechtenberg

January 17th, 2019

Perseverance isn’t down to personality: it can be developed

0 comments | 8 shares

Estimated reading time: 5 minutes

Berno Buechel

Lydia Mechtenberg

January 17th, 2019

Perseverance isn’t down to personality: it can be developed

0 comments | 8 shares

Estimated reading time: 5 minutes

The importance of perseverance is recognised by intellectuals, sportsmen, and business people alike:

  • Great works are performed not by strength but by perseverance.” (Samuel Johnson, author, 1709-1784)
  • I’ve missed more than 9000 shots in my career. […] I’ve failed over and over and over again in my life. And that is why I succeed.” (Michael Jordan, basketball player, *1963)
  • I do not think that there is any other quality so essential to success of any kind as the quality of perseverance.” (John D. Rockefeller, business magnate, 1839-1937)

What these quotes do not reveal, however, is whether perseverance is solely a matter of personality – which some lucky people own and others cannot acquire – or whether we can actually influence it. In our study, we investigate this simple, but critical question: Can you influence a peer’s perseverance? And how?

In short: yes, you can! And there are two distinct ways: learning and peer pressure.

Think of running a marathon

An amateur runner who aspires to complete a marathon can benefit from joining a group of experienced runners. One reason is peer pressure. Committing to regularly practicing together and announcing to the group one’s intention to run the marathon can help not to procrastinate. The other reason is learning. Listening to what experienced runners say and observing that they have succeeded finishing a marathon can boost one’s own perseverance, too. Yet, to choose a group of runners, it is important to know which effect is at work, under which conditions they are working, and when there are actually negative peer effects.

Tedious task versus online fun

In our study, we investigate both effects, learning and pressure, in a lab, where 336 subjects made anonymous decisions on computers. In order to challenge perseverance, we let subjects work on a tedious task (see Figure 1), while they could always surf the web instead. Participants received a monetary bonus for solving at least 5 out of 10 subtasks correctly in 50 minutes. To unlock each subtask, a website filled with eye-catching content covering many categories such as fun, sports, celebrities or news had to be passed such that there was always the tempting alternative to give up and surf the web.

Figure 1. A small example of the tedious task that requires perseverance

Notes: Solutions here are 2 and 11. In the experiment, there are ten subtasks each consisting of 99 words, which also include upper and lower cases. This task is particularly depleting, given many words contain almost the right pattern (e.g. “AbC”, but without being followed by a vowel).

Learning – encouraging and discouraging effects

In the “learning from peers” variation, participants interacted in a chat with an experienced peer prior to working on the task themselves. We found that chatting with a successful peer can have an encouraging or a discouraging effect on perseverance, depending on how this peer communicated. When the experienced peer had achieved the bonus herself and then communicated that most of the participants could probably do so, too, unexperienced chat partners were most likely to persevere until the end and achieved the bonus (see Figure 2). If, however, an experienced peer who had gotten the bonus then communicated that most of the other participants would not manage to succeed as well, this had a discouraging effect. It seemed to convey the message “I did it, but I am exceptionally good at this. You should not even try”. What about unsuccessful peers? We found that experienced peers who had themselves not gotten the bonus had no influence on others’ perseverance, regardless of what they communicated.

Figure 2. Learning from peers

Notes: Estimated probability of achieving the bonus is highest when a peer has achieved the bonus herself and she guessed that many participants would achieve it, too. Estimated probability of achieving the bonus is lowest when the peer has achieved the bonus herself and she guessed that only few participants would achieve it, too.

Peer pressure

In the “peer pressure” variation, each participant had to justify her performance to another participant, again in an online chat, but this time after the task. This variation also showed a significant impact on perseverance, but not a long-lasting one. Those who had some peer pressure managed more often to solve at least one or two subtasks (see Figure 3), but this did not lead to a larger fraction of participants going up to the bonus.

Figure 3. Peer pressure
Notes: Fraction who solved at least one subtask, respectively at least two subtasks, is higher when participants are observed by a peer to whom they have to justify their performance.

Gender differences

Interestingly, both types of peer effects, learning and pressure are significantly more pronounced for women in our data. Justifying the performance in front of a peer increases females’ average number of tasks solved from 2.74 to 3.97 while a positive successful peer’s recommendation to keep working increases the females’ average number of tasks solved from 0.71 to 4.24. In our experiment, it is hence the women who respond strongly to peer effects.

Implications for organisational design

These insights have suggestive implications for organisational design, in particular team composition and promotion decisions. If perseverance was solely a matter of personality, human resource departments could only try to select the right types, but not influence them. In contrast, our study has shown that there are simple ways we can positively affect someone’s perseverance. Beyond the rather obvious effects of pressure, it is possible to foster perseverance by a combination of exemplary success and humble communication. But beware: those who emphasise their own success rather than motivate others can have a discouraging effect. The additional observation that unsuccessful peers have no effect at all points to the necessity of legitimate authority for being able to give influential guidance. Finally, mentors and managers would be well advised to take the gender of those they advise into account: While it seems easier to motivate women, it also appears easier to demotivate them by picturing oneself as the exceptional achiever.

♣♣♣

Notes:

  • This blog post is based on the authors’ paperIf I can do it, so can you! Peer effects on perseverance“, with Julia Petersen, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, Volume 155, November 2018, Pages 301-314.
  • The post gives the views of its authors, not the position of LSE Business Review or the London School of Economics.
  • Featured image credit: Image by geralt, under a Pixabay licence
  • When you leave a comment, you’re agreeing to our Comment Policy.

Berno Buechel is a professor of economics at the University of Fribourg (Switzerland) and co-founder of the Swiss Center for Data + Network Sciences. His research focuses on the working of markets, on strategic behaviour, and on the role of social networks.

 

 

Lydia Mechtenberg is a professor of economics at the University of Hamburg (Germany) and work-package leader in the EU-Consortium DEMOS – Democratic Efficacy and the Varieties of Populism in Europe. She investigates the workings of markets, organisations, and institutions, using game theory and lab, field, and survey experiments.

About the author

Berno Buechel

Berno Buechel is a professor of economics at the University of Fribourg (Switzerland) and co-founder of the Swiss Center for Data + Network Sciences. His research focuses on the working of markets, on strategic behaviour, and on the role of social networks.

Lydia Mechtenberg

Lydia Mechtenberg is a professor of economics at the University of Hamburg (Germany) and work-package leader in the EU-Consortium DEMOS - Democratic Efficacy and the Varieties of Populism in Europe. She investigates the workings of markets, organisations, and institutions, using game theory and lab, field, and survey experiments.

Posted In: Career and Success

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.