LSE - Small Logo
LSE - Small Logo

Rebecca Sear

November 24th, 2023

The persistence of eugenics in mainstream journals highlights major gaps in research integrity

6 comments | 67 shares

Estimated reading time: 7 minutes

Rebecca Sear

November 24th, 2023

The persistence of eugenics in mainstream journals highlights major gaps in research integrity

6 comments | 67 shares

Estimated reading time: 7 minutes

When published, bad data can have long lasting negative impacts on research and the wider world. In this post Rebecca Sear, traces the impact of the national IQ dataset and reflects how its continued use in research highlights the lack of priority given to research integrity.


Would it surprise you that papers are regularly published in the academic literature that promote the idea that people in sub-Saharan Africa have cognitive ability so low that they are, on average, on the verge of intellectual impairment? Not just papers in ‘fringe’ journals, with little credibility, but in journals published by major academic publishers, Elsevier and Springer, and with influential and respected scientists on their editorial boards.

This blog tells the story of the national IQ dataset. This dataset claims to provide data on average cognitive ability in nation-states, yet in reality is scientifically flawed and has been used to explicitly argue for a genetically-determined racial hierarchy of intelligence. This research has not just been confined to academia, but has done real world harm; work using the dataset was cited in the manifesto of the terrorist who murdered 10 people in a racially motivated attack in Buffalo, US in 2022.

The idea of a racial hierarchy of intelligence is not new. It was an important component of the eugenics movement that had widespread support in the early 20th century. Eugenic ideology assumes human populations can be ‘improved’ through selective reproduction: encouraging those with ‘desirable’ traits to have children, while discouraging those with ‘undesirable’ traits from reproducing. Eugenics (ostensibly) fell out of favour during the late 20th century after the atrocities committed by the Nazi regime took eugenics to its ‘logical’ conclusion by murdering ‘undesirables’. However, this decline had started even before the 1940s, as it became increasingly clear that the simplistic assumptions on which eugenics is based did not hold true. Complex human traits and behaviours cannot easily be bred into (or out of) human populations. Eugenics is ideology, not science.

But eugenics never went away. Eugenics is underpinned by the belief that there are ‘hierarchies’ of humans and, in particular, hierarchies of race and class. One popular method eugenicists have used to identify such hierarchies is by measuring ‘intelligence’. Francis Galton – who coined the term ‘eugenics’ and originated the academic study of eugenics at University College London – pioneered the measurement of intelligence, to rank individuals on what he considered a ‘desirable’ trait.

In the 21st century, intelligence tests are still being used in support of eugenic ideology. In 2002, psychologist Richard Lynn published a dataset which he claims provides an average IQ (Intelligence Quotient) for each nation-state worldwide. This national IQ dataset has been widely used by Lynn and many other researchers, in >100 papers published across multiple disciplines including psychology, economics, biology and political science. This is very surprising if you consider that the average national IQ in the dataset for all sub-Saharan African countries combined is 70. A score so low that would imply large proportions of people in Africa have trouble leading ordinary lives without help.

In the 21st century, intelligence tests are still being used in support of eugenic ideology.

An inspection of Lynn’s dataset reveals why it comes up with such an implausible IQ for sub-Saharan Africa: it is bad science. Lynn calculated national IQs from data on cognitive tests already published in the scientific literature, but these studies largely involve samples that are wholly inappropriate for calculating national averages. Collecting data from a sample which is representative of a national population (in terms of age, sex, socioeconomic position, for example) is difficult and expensive. Lynn’s dataset instead relies on many ‘convenience’ samples, meaning samples which happened to be available: the IQ for Eritrea, for example, is estimated entirely from children living in orphanages. These samples are often very small: the national IQ of Angola is estimated from just 19 people.

The dataset is also systematically biased. No methodology for selecting samples from the published literature into the dataset has ever been provided, though it is clear that selections were made. A review of the literature on cognitive tests in sub-Saharan Africa found evidence for selection bias in that those samples which Lynn included for this region indicated lower cognitive ability than those excluded. Even if it were possible to put all these methodological flaws aside, it is simply not possible to generate a single figure for each nation’s ‘intelligence’ which is comparable worldwide. ‘Culture-fair’ cognitive tests do not exist; all such tests involve cultural assumptions, and will disadvantage people who have experienced little or no formal education.

Many academics have been involved in this exercise of embedding racist and eugenic ideology firmly into the scientific literature

Can the national IQ dataset really be considered science; research produced to help us find out about the world? The deeply flawed nature of the dataset suggests not. Lynn has also used the dataset to explicitly promote eugenic ideology. He claims that cognitive differences between world regions represent genetically-determined differences. He has linked IQ to skin colour, claiming Black populations are less intelligent than White or Asian populations (this is a scientifically meaningless statement given the consensus among geneticists and anthropologists that racial categories do not reflect meaningful genetic differences). And he has written about the policy implications of these differences; arguing, for example, that immigration from ‘low IQ’ regions of the world to ‘high IQ’ regions is problematic, as it will lead to a decline in the ‘quality’ of the latter populations.

There seems little doubt that the primary goal of this dataset is to promote an ideology of a racial hierarchy. Yet this dataset has now been used in dozens of academic publications, lending it scientific legitimacy, and producing research with highly problematic conclusions. Many academics have been involved in this exercise of embedding racist and eugenic ideology firmly into the scientific literature. Authors, reviewers and editors alike have either failed to notice, or were unconcerned about, the dataset’s flawed methods and absurd conclusions about African intelligence. Worse, attempts to keep this unsound work out of the literature consistently fail. Neither the many damning critiques of the dataset which have been published, nor a statement from an academic society circulated to some journal editors, have stopped its continued use.

academic freedom is the freedom to research any question of (social) scientific interest using appropriate standards of methodological rigour. Work which violates such standards should not be given a platform in academia

The continued circulation of the dataset may result from carelessness on the part of researchers, or from their implicit biases, but also from a confusion between academic freedom and freedom of speech. Some academics have made the naïve argument that flawed work should be published in the (social) scientific literature on the grounds of “academic freedom”. But, academic freedom is the freedom to research any question of (social) scientific interest using appropriate standards of methodological rigour. Work which violates such standards should not be given a platform in academia.

Academic structures also do little to incentivise research integrity. Incentives in academia largely favour the acquiring of research funds and publications, on the part of individual scientists, and the acquiring of profit, in the case of the largest academic publishers. Academics need to change these structures by prioritising research integrity in their own work, when reviewing and editing for journals, and when hiring, promoting and evaluating others. Until these structures shift, academia will remain vulnerable to manipulation by actors wishing to misuse science to promote political ideology.

 


This post draws on the author’s article, Demography and the rise, apparent fall, and resurgence of eugenics, published in Population Studies. 

The content generated on this blog is for information purposes only. This Article gives the views and opinions of the authors and does not reflect the views and opinions of the Impact of Social Science blog (the blog), nor of the London School of Economics and Political Science. Please review our comments policy if you have any concerns on posting a comment below.

Image Credit: Brina Blum via Unsplash.


Print Friendly, PDF & Email

About the author

Rebecca Sear

Rebecca Sear is Professor of Population and Health at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine.

Posted In: Academic publishing | Research ethics

6 Comments