LSE - Small Logo
LSE - Small Logo

Zoé Vanhersecke

October 11th, 2021

He’s a 10 outta 10, so is she, and you are too…

0 comments

Estimated reading time: 5 minutes

Zoé Vanhersecke

October 11th, 2021

He’s a 10 outta 10, so is she, and you are too…

0 comments

Estimated reading time: 5 minutes

At least from someone’s point of view.

When was the first time you rated someone’s attractiveness? Or should I rather ask, when was the last time? If you’re human you will likely have done this at least once, and for a good reason; it is part of what psychologists call the “mate choice”(Darwin, 1871; Jennions & Petrie, 1997; Miller & Todd, 1998; Miller, 2000; Kokko, Brooks, Jennions, & Morley, 2003; Geary, Vigil, & Byrd-Craven, 2004). In late adolescence, you begin taking part in this mating ‘game’, become more aware of your own body (McCandless, 1970; Schonfeld, 1969), and so attractiveness, to figure out your value on the ‘market’. Afterwards, you look at others, and try to snatch the best party you can get (and keep) (Kendrick et al., 1993)!

First, what do humans consider attractive?

The traits that humans look for in a partner can be separated into 3 categories:

  1. Species preferences, common to all. Natural selection brought people to find interest in them, as they indicate ‘fitness’ (Wallace, 2009). For example, waist-to-hip ratio in females, indicating fertility, is cross-culturally considered attractive (Singh 2006; Singh & Luis 1994; Streeter & McBurney 2003; Swami et al. 2007), and so is a thicker jawline in men, indicating masculinity, and thus a strong and healthy mate (Fink et al., 2007; Windhager et al., 2011).
  2. Cultural preferences, unique to each society [Illustration 1], and changing over time*.
  3. Personal preferences, coming from one’s genes and their interaction with the environment.

It should be noted, though, that humans judge in social comparison. One own’s image is dependent on a reference group (Gray, 2018). When rating others, the most well-known and easiest to access reference group to ourselves is ourselves. That would indicate that the base rate for rating attractiveness is one’s self, and others are either considered ‘above’ or ‘below’ on the scale.

Illustration 1. Source: Superdrug

But how do you find out your own value? Trial and error

Childhood and adolescence are commonly viewed as a learning period, and it isn’t only academics!

  1. Generally, flirting in the teenage years is a way for humans to develop their ‘Mating Intelligence’ (Furman, 2002), not so much finding long-lasting love. Through trial-and-error, you track your successes and rejections (Todd and Miller, 1999; Penke & Denissen, 2006) and figure out where you stand on the ‘mate value socio-meter’ (Kirkpatrick and Ellis, 2007). You adjust upward because you strive for the finest; and you adjust downward because getting rejected, as you surely know, hurts (Barkow, 1989; Dawkins, 1982; Kenrick et al., 1993; Sloman & Sloman, 1988; Trivers, 1972; Todd & Miller, 1999; Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). So, why does it hurt, you may ask? Self-esteem has evolved to preserve you from social exclusion (Leary and Baumeister, 2000). This means, the pain may be worth it so that you end up ‘playing in your own league’, settle with a partner (and reproduce).
  2. “Stop comparing yourself to others!” is now the mantra. However, comparison within your sex-group on traits relevant to dating also helps you assess your position in the game (Gutierres, Kenrick, & Partch, 1999; Brown, Novick, Lord, & Richards, 1992). So consume it, but in moderation!

Let’s put it in numbers

In today’s western societies, the somewhat questionable game of rating people’s physical appearance from 0 to 10 is quite popular.

People are also subject to the “better-than-average” effect (Svenson, 1981) in their perception of attractiveness. This bias was first discovered when almost everyone in the experiment thought of themselves being better drivers than the mean. Besides, the self-serving human does not want to hurt his self-esteem. Add to that, the desire to get a grade that is culturally considered ‘good’. And there you have everyone rating themselves a 7 out of 10!

So you think you’re a 7, and you rate your peers above or below you. Assume you and your friend have similar preferences when it comes to dating. You would rank a group of people (based on attractiveness) in the exact same order, perceive them in the exact same manner, but WITHOUT giving them the same actual grades.

How so? Looking at biases

Take Alex and Bob, sorting the same group of individuals. Both think of themselves as sevens, but Bob is more conventionally attractive than Alex.

A number of biases come into play. Both are subject to the “availability heuristic” (Tversky and Kahneman, 1973). They only consider the group at stake, or maybe their social circle, when judging; not the wide pool of human beings! Consequently, in a very good-looking group of friends, it may be more complicated to get a higher rating than in an average one. That is the “big- fish-in-small-pond effect” (Marsh et al., 2008) discovered when studying student’s varying self-concept, performing similarly, in a selective school versus a nonselective one.

Alex considers 50% of that group less attractive than himself, and will grade them from 0 to 7, he then has 50% of people left, to mark from 7 to 10. BUT Bob considers 70% of that group less attractive than him, which he will mark from 0 to 7. The proportion considered more attractive is then 30%, and those are the people he will grade from 7 to 10. Grades are then proportionally distributed within the subgroups.

The final question is: why ranking? What is the ultimate argument for it?

In evolutionary terms, across individuals:

  • grading someone from 0 to 7 is saying “mating with you is detrimental to me/degrades the genetic pool available to  my progeny”,
  • grading someone a 7 is saying “our genetic pools seem equal, mating with you is neither a gain nor a loss”,
  • and grading someone from 7 to 10 means “I’ll gladly mate with you, it would probably upgrade my offspring’s genetics”.

Take a look at the diagram [Illustration 3]. This means that Clyde (C), perceived as objectively attractive by both Alex (A) and Bob (B), would receive a grade 8 from Alex, and a 7 from Bob. The messages are respectively: “you are attractive, and more than I am” and “I do consider you attractive, but you are my equal”.

Illustration 3: Author’s own

So, the good news is, while you may be your high-school-cheerleader’s 3 out of 10 (the lowest grade I ever came across, people aren’t THAT rude), you are the ugly kid’s 10 outta 10.

P.S. But as we always say, beauty isn’t everything…Remember that not so attractive friend of yours, that somehow got all the boys and girls swarming over them? What do they have, that you don’t have? But maybe… could have?

Notes:

  • The views expressed in this post are of the author and not the Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science nor LSE.
  • This post was written as part of the course PB101: Foundations of Psychological Science, posted and edited with the permission of the author.
  • Illustration 1: Countries preferences of the female body and beauty; Source: Superdrug
  • Feature image: sourced via Pixabay.

References:

Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture (pp. xx, 453). University of Toronto Press.

Brown, P. J., & Konner, M. (2006). An Anthropological Perspective on Obesity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 499(1), 29–46. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb36195.x

Darwin, C. (1981). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Princeton University Press.

Denissen, J. J. A., Penke, L., Schmitt, D. P., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2008). Self-esteem reactions to social interactions: Evidence for sociometer mechanisms across days, people, and nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 181–196. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.181

Dixson, B. J. W., Sulikowski, D., Gouda-Vossos, A., Rantala, M. J., & Brooks, R. C. (2016). The masculinity paradox: Facial masculinity and beardedness interact to determine women’s ratings of men’s facial attractiveness. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 29(11), 2311–2320. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/jeb.12958

Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19(1), 82–87. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20583

Flinn, M. V. (1984). The extended phenotype: The gene as the unit of selection. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5(3), 215–217. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/0162-3095(84)90028-1

Furman, W. (2002). The Emerging Field of Adolescent Romantic Relationships. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 177–180. https:// doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00195

Geary, D. C., Vigil, J., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2004). Evolution of human mate choice. Journal of Sex Research, 41(1), 27–42. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/00224490409552211

Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. M. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press.

Gray, P. O., & Bjorklund, D. (2018). Psychology (8th ed.). Macmillan International Higher Education.

Gutierres, S. E., Kenrick, D. T., & Partch, J. J. (1999). Beauty, Dominance, and the Mating Game: Contrast Effects in Self-Assessment Reflect Gender Differences in Mate Selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(9), 1126–1134. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672992512006

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/ 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

Jennions, M. D., & Petrie, M. (2007). VARIATION IN MATE CHOICE AND MATING PREFERENCES: A REVIEW OF CAUSES AND CONSEQUENCES. Biological Reviews, 72(2), 283–327. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.1997.tb00015.x

Kenrick, D. T., Groth, G. E., Trost, M. R., & Sadalla, E. K. (1993). Integrating evolutionary and social exchange perspectives on relationships: Effects of gender, self-appraisal, and involvement level on mate selection criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 951–969. https:// doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.951

Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Ellis, B. J. (2007). An Evolutionary-Psychological Approach to Self-esteem: Multiple Domains and Multiple Functions. In G. J. O. Fletcher & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Interpersonal Processes (pp. 409–436). Blackwell Publishers Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470998557.ch16

Kokko, H., Brooks, R., Jennions, M. D., & Morley, J. (2003). The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 270(1515), 653–664. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2235

Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 1–62). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9

Lerner, R. M., & Karabenick, S. A. (1974). Physical attractiveness, body attitudes, and self-concept in late adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 3(4), 307–316. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02214744

Marsh, H. W., Seaton, M., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Hau, K. T., O’Mara, A. J., & Craven, R. G. (2008). The Big-fish–little-pond-effect Stands Up to Critical Scrutiny: Implications for Theory, Methodology, and Future Research. Educational Psychology Review, 20(3), 319–350. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s10648-008-9075-6

McCandless, B. R. (1970). Adolescents: Behavior and development. Dryden Press.

Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature (1st ed). Doubleday.

Miller, G. F., & Todd, P. M. (1998). Mate choice turns cognitive. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(5), 190–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S1364-6613(98)01169-3

Moore, C. R. (1951). Patterns of Sexual Behavior. Clellan S. Ford and Frank A. Beach. New York: Harper, 1951. 307 pp. $4.50. Science, 114(2955), 190–191. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.114.2955.190-a

Morris, A., Cooper, T., & Cooper, P. J. (1989). The changing shape of female fashion models. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8(5), 593– 596. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(198909)8:5<593::AID-EAT2260080511>3.0.CO;2-H

Penke, L., Todd, P. M., Lenton, A. P., & Fasolo, B. (2008). How self-assessments can guide human mating decisions. In Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. (pp. 37–75). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

*Romero, M. (n.d.). Male Body Ideals Through Time. Retrieved December 5, 2020, from https://lammily.com/magazine/male-body-ideals-through-time/

Schonfeld, W. A. (1964). Body-image disturbances in adolescents with inappropriate sexual development. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 34(3), 493–502. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1964.tb02218.x

Singh, D. (2006). Universal Allure of the Hourglass Figure: An Evolutionary Theory of Female Physical Attractiveness. Clinics in Plastic Surgery, 33(3), 359–370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cps.2006.05.007

Singh, D., & Luis, S. (1995). Ethnic and gender consensus for the effect of waist-to-hip ratio on judgment of women’s attractiveness. Human Nature, 6(1), 51–65. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02734135

Singh, D., & Young, R. K. (1995). Body weight, waist-to-hip ratio, breasts, and hips: Role in judgments of female attractiveness and desirability for relationships. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(6), 483–507. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(95)00074-7

Sloman, S., & Sloman, L. (1988). Mate selection in the service of human evolution. Journal of Social and Biological Systems, 11(4), 457–468. https:// doi.org/10.1016/0140-1750(88)90083-8

Streeter, S. (2003). Waist–hip ratio and attractiveness New evidence and a critique of “a critical test.” Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 88–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00121-6

Superdrug. (n.d.). Perceptions of Perfection Across Borders. Retrieved December 5, 2020, from https://onlinedoctor.superdrug.com/perceptions-of-perfection/

Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychologica, 47(2), 143–148. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/0001-6918(81)90005-6

Swami, V., Neto, F., Tovée, M. J., & Furnham, A. (2007). Preferences for Female Body Weight and Shape in Three European Countries. European Psychologist, 12(3), 220–228. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.12.3.220

Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Barrett, H. (2005). Resolving the debate on innate ideas: Learnability constraints and the evolved interpenetration of motivational and conceptual functions. Undefined. /paper/Resolving-the-debate-on-innate-ideas%3A-Learnability-Tooby-Cosmides/ 1a1c81b08e482a1566555edb46810e327a304373

Trivers, R. (1972). Parental Investment and Sexual Selection. In Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man (p. 378).

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232. https:// doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9

Wallace, A. R. (2009). Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection, with Some of its Applications. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511693076

Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23(6), 805–814. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.21219

About the author

Zoé Vanhersecke

Zoé is a BSc Psychological and Behavioural Science student and Research Assistant at the LSE. The plurality of psychology fascinates her, as well as how useful it is to explain human’s behaviour and conception of the world. Eventually, she hopes to be able to use her knowledge to solve everyday problems.

Posted In: BSc | BSc Psychological and Behavioural Science | Cultural Evolution | PB101 Foundations of Psychological Science | Psychology | Social psychology

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.